Date Published: July
22, 2014
Source: Behavioral
Ecology
Institutions involved: University
of Konstanz; Department of Biology, Planck Institute for Ornithology; School of
Human Evolution & Social Change; Arizona State University.
Summary: A study
conducted by M. Teague O’Mara, Dina K.N.
Dechmann, and Rachel A. Page in the Behavioral Ecology journal proposes
the possibility of social interaction having a great impact on what food a
species of bats decide to eat.
A
study conducted by M. Teague O’Mara,
Dina K.N. Dechmann, and Rachel A. Page in the journal of Behavioral Ecology proposes
the possibility of social cues having a great impact on what food a species of
bats decide to eat. Published July 22, 2012, the journal goes into depth about
the different social interactions observed among the Uroderma bilobatum species
of bats. Furthermore, several conclusions are drawn about what happens when a
bat returns to the roost having eaten a new or novel food.
Researchers
have long hypothesized, and now theorized, that animals have the ability to
gather information from other animals that enter or pass by the roost. Although
true, what is incredible is not
that they have been found to have this ability, but that they can differentiate
between reliable and unreliable sources in the form of cues. When individuals
come into the roost, they exhibit a sort of signal, or cue, to its roostmates indicating
that it had just eaten. These cues could potentially be a change in weight,
increased grooming, or odor on breath or fur of the individual. A group of
researchers found that these cues can affect the feeding choices of other
individuals for a significant amount of time.
The method involved novel
flavored bananas, as the bats are used to a diet of banana. The Uroderma
bilobatum were captured and recorded for their mass, forearm length, reproductive
status, age, and sex. They were then tagged ad monitored daily for the purpose
of their well-being. All of the participants came from the same roost to guarantee
their familiarity with one another. The four experiments tested the reaction of
bats to novel odor exposure, social information transfer in captivity and in
nature, and the ability to discriminate information given by two demonstrators.
The first experiment conducted
by O'Mara and his team involved a dish of novel flavoring mixed with banana
that was placed in a mesh enclosure, so that the individual bats could smell
but not access it for the purpose of familiarization. The bats were then
offered two dishes of food, one with the "familiar" smell and another
novel flavored "unfamiliar" dish. In the second experiment, a few
bats were taken out of their cage, fed a flavored sugar solution and returned
to their cage to eat more bananas. The demonstrator bats were individually
given a choice of either bananas with the same novel flavor or a new flavor.
The third experiment began with the capture of several free range bats. They
were held in a cage and fed a flavored banana-sugar water mixture then freed
after two days. The group of researchers then captured members of the roosts
they captured the demonstrator bats from and were offered bananas flavored with
their demonstrator's flavor or another flavor. In the fourth experiment, O'Mara
and his team fed two demonstrators, the first was fed novel-flavored bananas
and the second was fed an unflavored sugar solution and then a novel-flavored banana
juice mixture was applied to its fur.
The
team found that interaction with a novel food odor wasn't enough for a food
bias to form. Furthermore, observer bats were more likely to consume food eaten
by demonstrator bats earlier. The fundamental finding is that information
gathered from cues was likely to remain in a roost for a few days afterward. This
ties into what researchers already know; animals interact and provide one
another with information about their environment to continue the struggle to
survive.
CITATION: O'Mara, M.T, D.K.N Dechmann, and R.A Page.
2014. Frugivorous bats evaluate the quality of social information when choosing
novel foods. Behavioral Ecology 25(5): 1233–1239.
The fact that the bats work together to figure out what to eat is really cool. I wonder if they could tell that food is bad to eat from the other bats in their roost. In the experiment, the researchers only used different novel flavors. I think that if they put old food, or food that would harm the bats in a dish, none of the bats would eat it.
ReplyDelete